Gender differences
on crime and punishment.
Author: Hurwitz, Jon.; Smithey, Shannon. Source:
Political Research Quarterly
v. 51 no1 (Mar. 1998) p. 89-115 ISSN: 1065-9129 Number:
BSSI98016878 Copyright: The magazine publisher is the
copyright holder of this article and it is reproduced with
permission. Further reproduction of this article in
violation of the copyright is prohibited.
Since the 1980 election, considerable political science
attention has been devoted to the gap between women's and
men's political preferences and voting behavior. Although
the differences between the sexes have tended to be fairly
small in empirical terms,(FN1) they have appeared across a
consistent range of issues. At the same time, feminist
theory has been exploring the implications of Carol
Gilligan's In a Different Voice (1982). Gilligan's
suggestion that women's psychology is premised on an
"ethic of care" rather than on an "ethic of
justice" has been applied in numerous contexts because
it seems to fit with the differences found in women's and
men's approaches to a range of topics.(FN2).
Gender differences have been well-established over a wide
array of public policy attitudes and behaviors, including
policies promoting government caretaking of vulnerable
citizens (Conover 1994; Thomas 1994; Rhine et al. 1994;
Mueller 1988; Stoper 1988), military commitments (Conover
and Sapira 1993; Shapiro and Mahajan 1986; Smith 1984), and
voting (Bedyna and Lake 1994; Gilens 1988; Pomper 1975).
Less is known, however, about male-female differences on one
of the most salient "hot button" issues--crime.
This gap is surprising for two reasons: first, there are
important theoretical reasons to presuppose gender-based
differences in the ways men and women respond to crime; and
second, as we shall argue in the conclusions, crime is a
strategically important issue. The failure to appreciate
gender-related differences pertaining to crime, therefore,
undercuts the efforts of candidates to turn the issue to
partisan advantage.
In this article we systematically examine how, and why,
women and men approach issues of crime and punishment. Our
first purpose is to explore the specific areas in which the
genders differ. Next, we consider two competing explanations
of these differences. The first of these hypotheses relates
crime and punishment attitudes to gender differences in
perceptions of vulnerability. The second links these
attitudes with the "different voices" approach
espoused by Gilligan. Our findings support the idea that the
gender gap in crime and punishment attitudes owes a great
deal to differences in men and women's caretaking
orientations rather than emerging solely from differences in
their fears of victimization.
PERCEPTIONS OF VULNERABILITYAttitudes toward crime and
punishment are often thought to flow from feelings of
vulnerability. It would come as no surprise to find that
people who are more afraid of crime are also more supportive
of efforts to prevent it. For example, individuals who feel
particularly vulnerable might be more prone to favor
increasing the power of the police or passing laws to make
guns harder to obtain. Those who are less afraid may be less
likely to see the need for such measures.
Perceptions of vulnerability may explain orientations
toward punishment as well as prevention. If harsher
penalties are thought to keep the public safer by deterring
potential offenders,(FN3) we would expect to find people's
support for aggressive responses to crime to increase along
with their level of fear. Indeed, studies of attitudes
toward crime almost all proceed from the notion that fear of
victimization increases harshness (for examples, see Miller,
Rossi, and Simpson 1986; Research and Forecasts 1983;
Stinchcombe et al. 1980). On this basis, those who see
themselves as potential victims are expected to support
policies such as longer prison terms and capital punishment
more than those who feel more secure.
If perceived vulnerability is the key to these attitudes,
we would expect to find a gender gap in public responses to
crime and punishment issues. Women are made to feel more
vulnerable than men in a number of areas of life. Women have
traditionally controlled a smaller share of political and
economic resources than men (see for example Goldberg and
Kremen 1994). Large numbers of working women are
concentrated into lower paying, low prestige jobs, resulting
in widespread feelings of economic vulnerability (see, for
example, Sidel 1986). Women are also more likely than men to
need public assistance, especially if they are raising
children as single parents (see, for example Norris 1985).
Women's lives are also profoundly influenced by the
threat of physical violence, particularly domestic abuse and
rape, in ways that men's are not (Ferraro 1995; Walklate
1995; Gordon and Riger 1989). And, women are much more
likely than men to modify their behavior because they fear
victimization (Ferraro 1995; Walklate 1995; Gordon and Riger
1989). Stanko (1990: 85) explains this as a response to
women's heightened sense of vulnerability:.
Women's lives rest upon a continuum of unsafety. This
does not mean that all women occupy the same position in
relation to safety and violence.... Somehow, though, as all
women reach adulthood, they share a common awareness of
their particular vulnerability. Learning the strategies for
survival is a continuous lesson about what it means to be
female.
Conversely, men tend to have greater confidence in their
ability to protect themselves (Stanko 1990). This may
explain why surveys invariably report greater fear of crime
among women than men (Walklate 1995; Ferraro 1995; Karmen
1991; Stanko 1990; Gordon and Riger 1989; Research and
Forecasts Inc. 1983; Stinchcombe et al. 1980).
Women's greater sense of vulnerability therefore seems
likely to produce a gender gap in crime and punishment
attitudes. If fear is the primary determinant of such
attitudes, women should be particularly supportive of crime
prevention policies. Women's greater fear may also lead them
to be more punitive than men, especially in response to
crimes where woman are particularly vulnerable, such as rape
or domestic abuse. This would be consistent with Gordon and
Riger's (1989: 131) finding that "many women think rape
might be deterred by meting out harsher penalties to
convicted rapists," and Pierce and Harris's (1993)
findings that women were more likely than men to advocate
conviction for wife batterers. Given women's particular role
in caring for children, we might also expect women to react
with greater punitiveness when confronting crimes of
violence against children.
A few studies have found women to be more supportive than
men of some crime prevention policies, although there is not
a great deal of evidence on this point. At least two surveys
have found that women are less supportive of civil liberties
which inhibit the ability of the police to catch criminals
(Rhine et al. 1994; Shapiro and Mahajan 1986). Women also
support gun control measures at higher rates than men do
(Shapiro and Mahajan 1986; Smith 1984). It may be that men
are less supportive of these measures because they feel less
need for protection from crime. This relationship is
unclear, however, because the link between fear and support
for prevention policies has generally been assumed rather
than tested.
The empirical relationship between fear and punitiveness
is even less clear. Though many studies have concluded that
punitiveness varies with a person's fear of crime, their
data reveal a connection for men but not for women (see, for
example, Ferraro 1995; Mills and Bohannon 1992; Stinchcombe
et al. 1980). In fact, men, with their lower levels of fear,
are often more supportive of harsher penalties. For example,
men are more likely than women to support the use of capital
punishment for murderers (Conover 1994; Shapiro and Mahajan
1986; Smith 1984; Stinchcombe et al. 1980). On the other
hand, women do favor more punitive responses to rape and
domestic abuse, and female jurors are more likely to convict
those charged with violent crimes (Mills and Bohannon 1992).
It is clear that further exploration of the relationship
between gender, perceived vulnerability, and the desire to
punish offenders is required.
CRIME AND THE ETHIC OF CAREIt is likely that factors
other than mere fear, particularly orientations toward
violence and its prevention, play a key role in
distinguishing men's attitudes from women's toward crime and
punishment. Considerations of crime bring up issues of
vulnerability, but they also raise the specter of violence.
Women tend to view violence of all sorts more negatively
than men do. For example, women are less accepting of
violence on television, corporal punishment in schools, and
casualties in wartime (Conover and Sapiro 1993; Smith 1984).
Women are also more, likely than men to rate violent
behavior as seriously criminal (Mills and Bohannon
1992).(FN4) Men are more likely to support policies, such as
military spending and military intervention, which involve
the use of force. Men are also more likely to choose force
in response to public disturbances (Blumenthal et al. 1972)
and to condone use of force by police to subdue suspects
(Smith 1984). It may be that women's greater discomfort with
violence provides a common theme in their greater fear of
crime, their greater support for crime prevention measures,
and their resistance to harsher punishments.
Gender differences in tolerance for violence relate
directly to Carol Gilligan's theory of "different
voices" in moral reasoning. Based on her
dissatisfaction with earlier studies of moral
development,(FN5) Gilligan argues that there are two basic
moral perspectives, or "voices." Men's responses
to moral quandaries tend to conform to an "ethic of
justice," which emphasizes individual rights and relies
on "absolute rules of truth and fairness" (Gilligan
1982: 166). Conversely, women's responses tend to suggest an
"ethic of care," in which the focus is on
"society as an interdependent and interconnected web of
personal relationships; from this perspective moral or just
behavior entails actively attempting to nurture and protect
others and relationships among them" (Worden 1993:
206).
At the base of the ethic of care is concern for
protecting the vulnerable. From this perspective, the
morally responsible person considers the impact of her
actions on those around her, beginning always with the
"premise that no one should be hurt" (Bender 1988:
31). In this respect, Gilligan's theory of different voices
suggests that there may be more to men's and women's
attitudes toward crime than personal vulnerability because
preventing crime is an important way to protect others from
harm.
Gilligan and others who argue for the ethic of care
attribute men and women's different voices to socialization
experiences (see, for example, Gilligan 1993; Brody
1990).(FN6) They view gender differences as socially
constructed, rather than as essential sex traits. Crawford
(1995: 16) explains the process of social construction this
way: "Each of us believes in gendered ways because we
are placed in gendered social contexts. Women encounter
different social contexts from men. Women and men face
different expectations and norms even for what look like
identical situations.".
Female socialization experiences stress connection and
concern for others from early childhood on (Belenky et al.
1986; Chodorow 1978). Brody (1990: 80), for example, argues:
"girls are encouraged to identify with, and be
dependent on, their mothers. As the ones who are taught to
be the nurturers, nurses, homemakers, and 'kinkeepers in the
family they constantly receive signals that they should be
like their mothers. Boys, on the other hand, are encouraged
to be instrumental and active like their fathers." As a
result, women tend to see strength in connection and
nurturance, rather than in assertion or aggression (Gilligan
1982: 167-68).
In contrast, boys' socialization stresses separation,
independence, and autonomy (Chodorow 1978; 167). Schools
encourage boys to be more assertive and achievement-oriented
than girls (Bailey et al. 1992). Boys are also socialized to
be more competitive; rather than concern for attachment,
there is concern for "who's better than who, who's
bigger than who, who's stronger than who" (Stanko
19901: 110). In addition, boys tend to be disciplined with
more "power-oriented techniques" (Mussen and
Eisenberg-Berg 1977: 67) and are more routinely exposed to
more aggressive behavior, even violence on playgrounds and
in sports. They are expected to cope with it by becoming
stronger, more self-reliant. In the process, they learn the
lesson that "safety comes from strength, backed up by
physical ability" (Stanko 1990: 110).
Adult experiences also contribute to gender differences.
Men are expected to be self-reliant, and they are generally
slower to recognize social interdependence (Doyle and Paludi
1991; McClelland 1975). Women's worldviews are shaped by
their greater caregiving responsibilities, for children (Ruddick
1989; Elshtain 1981; Chodorow 1978) and for elderly and
disabled relatives (Hooyman and Gonyea 1995; Brody 1990). As
Hooyman and Gonyea (1995: 22-23) note, "male-female
differences exist in caregiving within the home and the paid
workforce, not simply because men and women are treated
differently as children, but because they also confront
different gender-based socially structured opportunities as
adults." These experiences have a significant impact
that continues to influence men and women in different ways.
Differences in orientation are apparent by gender in
attitudes toward a variety of public policy issues. Women
are consistently more supportive of "compassion"
policies in ways that conform to the ethic of care. Women
express greater sympathy for the disadvantaged (Conover
1994), leading greater numbers of women to favor public
expenditures for social welfare, education, and health care
(Rhine et al. 1994; Stoper 1988; Burt 1986; Shapiro and
Mahajan 1986). Related to this concern for the disadvantaged
is women's heightened sensitivity to inequality based on
race and ethnicity, which leads women to be more supportive
of government efforts to protect racial and ethnic
minorities (Rhine et al. 1994). Women are also more likely
than men to support environmental regulations, the 55 mile
an hour speed limit, mandatory seat belt use, and consumer
protection laws (Shapiro and Mahajan 1986). All these
programs are designed to afford protection and avoid harm,
which links them thematically to the caretaking that
underlies women's greater support for compassion
policies.(FN7).
We might expect gender differences toward crime and
punishment policies to reflect similar themes. Efforts to
prevent crime tap into the same constellation of values that
underlie the ethic of care. Controlling crime is one of the
best ways to protect the vulnerable and prevent
victimization. Crime is also a serious disturbance in the
web of relational connections; it threatens the safety of
neighborhoods that Shapiro and Mahajan (1986) find to be so
important to women's policy preferences. This makes it
likely that women, with their greater concern for preventing
harm, will put the most emphasis on prevention as the answer
to crime. Stopping crime before it starts is the best way to
keep people from getting hurt.
For many men, on the other hand, the ethic of justice is
premised on individual rights and conformity to rules.
Efforts to prevent crimes, such as gun control or expanding
police powers, may conflict with the ethic of justice's
emphasis on individual autonomy by allowing the state to
interfere before a law has been broken. Thus, the different
voices perspective suggests that women should be more
supportive of crime prevention efforts than men.
Yet, while we have good reason to expect women to be more
prevention-oriented than men, gender differences in the area
of punishment are less easy to predict. On the one hand,
given socialization differences, we might expect men to be
more punitive than women in response to crime. Men are
socialized to be more comfortable with aggression. Boys are
more likely to experience power-oriented punishment than
girls, and they learn to prove themselves through shows of
physical force. And, the ethic of justice emphasizes the
need for punishment when the rules are broken: criminals
violate the rules, so they must pay a penalty. We also know
that men are more comfortable than women with public policy
options, such as the commitment of troops, which are
premised on the use of violence. Thus, men's greater comfort
with aggression, in combination with women's greater
distaste for violence, might lead men to be more punitive
than women.
There are, on the other hand, sound theoretical reasons
to presuppose that gender differences toward punishment may
vary contextually. For example, we might expect women to
react more punitively when confronting crimes of violence
against children, for whom they have particular caretaking
responsibilities. They may also advocate responding with
greater force to crimes where women are particularly
vulnerable, such as rape or domestic abuse. This would be
consistent with the findings of Gordon and Riger (1989) and
Pierce and Harris (1993) cited above. There are, then,
reasons to expect a certain amount of punitiveness from both
men and women, according to Gilligan's perspective.
As a caveat, it is important to remember, in exploring
gender differences, that the attitudinal variations between
men and women are matters of emphasis and degree. There are
significant differences between men's and women's attitudes,
but these differences are often not particularly large. As
Conover and Sapiro (1993: 1095) note, these differences are
not large enough to divide women and men into different
camps. And, they certainly are not large enough to warrant
making the kinds of sweeping statements differentiating
women and men that have long been part of the stereotype.
It is nevertheless true that there are important
differences between the sexes that need to be understood.
The majority of both sexes may support a particular
candidate or policy, but they may do so for different
reasons or with varying degrees of intensity. For example,
Klein (1984: 155) found that majorities of both men and
women voted for Carter in the 1976 presidential election,
but that they did so for different reasons. She also reports
that majorities of both sexes were opposed to government
provision of jobs and aid to minorities, but that women were
less likely to emphasize self-reliance than men. Similarly,
Conover and Sapiro (1993: 1092) found that majorities of
both men and women were disgusted by the killing involved in
the Persian Gulf but men were less intense in their
opposition. We expect to find similar differences of degree
in men's and women's approaches to crime and punishment.
METHODS, DATA, AND INSTRUMENTATIONIn the analysis which
follows, we first examine gender differences in three areas:
fear of crime, attitudes toward crime prevention policies,
and attitudes toward punishment for offenders. Importantly,
the differences which we subsequently document cannot,
alone, provide much leverage with respect to the two
explanations discussed above--i.e., vulnerability vs.
socialization. First, both of these perspectives predict
women to be more supportive of crime prevention policies,
albeit for somewhat different reasons. Second, while the
vulnerability explanation certainly assumes that women would
be more fearful of crime, such a finding would, in no way,
be inconsistent with a socialization perspective, such as
that articulated by Gilligan. And third, the expectations of
both perspectives lead us to be essentially agnostic with
regard to punishment: the empirical linkage between fear and
punitiveness is far from clear, as is the theoretical
relationship between caretaking and punishment. The first
section of the analysis, therefore, will be more descriptive
than analytical. In the second portion, however, we turn to
an analysis of the impact of fear on crime policy attitudes,
attempting to address the relative power of these two
explanations.
For purposes of analysis, we use data from a probability
survey conducted by the University of Kentucky Survey
Research Center (UKSRC), which interviewed 501 adults in the
greater Lexington, Kentucky area.(FN8) Respondents were
selected through a variant of random digit dialing
procedures between June 22 and July 5, 1994, and were
interviewed by the professional staff at UKSRC for
approximately 15 to 20 minutes on matters of crime. Because
the survey was initially designed to examine whites'
attitudes toward race and crime, only white adults
participated.(FN9) All survey items were pretested with a
smaller nonprobability sample (N = 410) in April 1994,
leading to extensive modifications in the survey items
appearing on the final instrument.(FN10).
Given the composition of our sample, we can only be
confident that our findings apply to Caucasians. However, we
would not be surprised to find a similar gender gap among
non-whites. Women of all races are socialized to place
greater emphasis on caregiving. We might therefore expect
women of all races to be particularly concerned with crime
and its prevention. This seems to be borne out by studies
such as those by Mills and Bohannon (1992), Miller et al.
(1986), and Rossi et. al. (1974), which find that black
women consider crime to be a more serious problem than black
men do. Our data do not permit an analysis of gender
differences among non-whites; our findings may nonetheless
suggest possible paths of analysis for others to study.
MEASURESCrucial to this analysis are measures which tap
feelings of personal safety and policy attitudes toward
crime.(FN11) The four Safety items (Appendix A, Items A.1-4)
have been recorded so that maximum values reflect feelings
of vulnerability and perceptions of increasing crime rates.
The Lexington survey also includes a series of Crime Policy
questions, measuring support for both preventive and
punitive solutions to crime. Items B.5-8 tap respondent
support for crime prevention policies, including spending
money for job training programs, creating drug
rehabilitation facilities, restricting hand gun purchases,
and conducting door-to-door searches for illegal drugs and
weapons. While these four policies are designed to prevent
crimes from occurring, the four criminal punishment policy
items (B.9-12) address policies that are intended to punish
the perpetrators once crimes have been committed. Thus, we
question respondents about their attitudes toward longer
prison sentences for violent criminals, the death penalty,
building more prisons, and mandatory life sentences for
those who sell drugs to children. Lower scale values on each
of these eight items reflect stronger support for the
policy, regardless of whether it is preventive or punitive
in nature. Finally, the last three items B.13-15) provide an
explicit choice to respondents between preventive and
punitive policies: between reducing poverty and
"cracking down on criminals" (B.13), between
rehabilitation and punishment of criminals (B.14), and
between social programs and more forceful policing to
prevent rioting (B.15). For these last three measures, lower
values represent stronger support for more punitive
preferences.
ANALYSISAs a first step in the investigation of gender
differences, we conduct a series of difference-of-means (t)
tests between women and men, the results of which are
reported in Table 1. Quite clearly, there is much to suggest
heightened feelings of vulnerability among female
respondents, as indicated in the first block of items in the
table. Women are significantly more likely than men to feel
unsafe in their neighborhoods, to judge crime as a more
important issue, and to believe there has been a large
increase in the nation's violent crime rate over the past
five years. While men and women do not differ with regard to
perceptions of increases in crime within their own
neighborhoods, the differences on the other items,
particularly the first, are notable.
It is with the policy items that we find the most
interesting differences between genders. We have defined the
first four policy measures (5-8) as "preventive,"
inasmuch as they are designed to minimize the likelihood of
crimes being committed. In each of these cases, as
anticipated, women are significantly more supportive: they
are more likely to favor spending money on job and drug
rehabilitation programs, restricting hand guns, and
conducting door-to-door searches for drugs and weapons.
Differences on the last item, quite possibly, are at least
partially a byproduct of the tendency for women to be
somewhat less civil libertarian relative to men (e.g.,
Shapiro and Mahajan 1986).
While women are significantly more supportive of
preventive policies across the board, the same cannot be
said of their responses to punitive policies (items 9-12).
Plainly, the gender differences on both increasing prison
terms for those who commit violent crimes and on building
more prisons are both insignificant and minimal. And while
significant gender differences emerge on the other two
items, it is impossible to recognize a consistent pattern,
inasmuch as men express more punitive responses to the death
penalty question, but less punitive responses when asked
about mandatory life prison terms for those who sell drugs
to children.
Thus, while women have been found to be universally more
supportive of preventive anti-crime policies, gender
patterns on punitive measures are clearly context specific.
The contexts, moreover, appear consistent with what has been
learned about the more general predispositions and concerns
of males and females. Numerous studies (e.g., Conover 1994;
Shapiro and Mahajan 1986; Smith 1984; and Stinchcombe et al.
1980), as noted, found men to be more supportive of capital
punishment, a tendency which, according to many, stems from
a more general support among men for policies which involve
the use of force or violence. On the other hand, Conover
(1994), among others, has also found women to be more
sympathetic toward the disadvantaged and the
vulnerable--groups which would certainly include children.
To find that women are more supportive of life terms for
those who sell drugs to children, consequently, suggests
that women have the capacity to be more punitive than men,
at least when the infraction victimizes an individual or
group dependent on the care-taking of women.
This tendency, wherein women are more likely than men to
advocate preventive (but not punitive) policy measures, is
seen most explicitly in two of the final items of Table 1,
both of which pose trade-offs between the utility of social
programs to prevent crime versus "get tough"
policies to punish perpetrators. Compared to males, females
are significantly more likely to see poverty reduction
(rather than "cracking down on criminals") as the
better solution to crime and, as well, are more likely to
feel that riots, such as those occurring in Los Angeles
prior to the survey, are better prevented by improved social
programs than by a greater use of force.
The differences which we initially anticipated, for the
most part, have received empirical support, as women have
been found to feel more vulnerable to crime and to be more
likely to support preventive anti-crime measures. We also
find that men and women do not differ consistently with
regard to punitive policy items, quite possibly because
women are conflicted in their beliefs. On the one hand, they
are more concerned with crime and more likely to want to
correct the problem. On the other hand, however, they are
more likely to resist the use of force, as suggested by
their stronger opposition to the death penalty.
MULTIVARIATE ANALYSISFindings, to this point, must be
regarded as preliminary, inasmuch as more fully specified
models may reveal the gender-based differences to be
spurious. We know, for example, that liberals (and, to a
lesser extent, Democrats), tend to be more supportive of
preventive policies, and less supportive of punitive
policies, relative to conservatives (and Republicans). To
the degree that gender is correlated with ideology and
partisanship, then, it is conceivable that ideology, rather
than gender, is the basis for the attitudinal differences
found to this point. Older respondents, moreover, tend to be
more fearful of crime in our sample; given the slightly
older age of female respondents relative to males,(FN12) it
is possible that age, rather than gender, motivates the
higher levels of fear reported by women.
Of equal concern is the possibility that the gender
differences reported in Table 1 are reflective of potential
social status distinctions between men and women. Wealthier
and better educated individuals tend to feel safer and less
concerned with crime (doubtless because they do not often
live in the higher crime neighborhoods). Inasmuch as female
respondents are both less affluent and less politically
knowledgeable than males,(FN13) it is important to control
for socioeconomic variables in analyzing gender differences
on crime.
To examine the relationship between gender and anti-crime
attitudes more rigorously, we constructed regression
equations for all the items for which there were significant
differences by gender. In addition to gender, we included
measures of ideology, party identification, age, education,
political sophistication, and income,(FN14) as independent
variables--variables which, for reasons noted above could
potentially alter the zero-order relationships found in
Table 1. In Table 2, column 1, we report the unstandardized
regression coefficients for the gender dummy variables,
which are interpretable as the mean differences between
males and females for each item, while holding constant
levels of political predisposition (ideology and party),
age, education, political knowledge, and affluence.
Despite the introduction of other controls, gender
continues to be a significant predictor of most attitudes
toward crime and punishment. The gender differences reported
in Table 1 remain, with only two exceptions: we no longer
find significant differences between men and women on the
life-prisonterm item, and on the
importance-of-crime-as-a-problem item. Otherwise, all
differences revealed in the initial t-tests remain
statistically significant when controlling for a host of
political and demographic variables.
The full regression models (see Appendix B) demonstrate
the true importance of gender in shaping crime policy
attitudes. It can be seen from the standardized coefficients
that, not only does gender remain statistically significant
in nine of the eleven equations, it is also the single
strongest determinant in five of the models and among the
most important predictors in most of the others. This is not
to suggest that other factors play no role: clearly older
age serves to make respondents feel more vulnerable to crime
(see equations 1, 2, and 4), and liberals (and, to a lesser
extent, Democrats) consistently endorse preventive policies
while opposing punitive policies. Other variables, moreover,
are selectively important: affluence, for example, is the
strongest determinant of support for the death penalty. It
is clearly the case, however, that no other variable plays
as consistently an important role in shaping crime policy
attitudes (and feelings of vulnerabilityto crime) as does
gender.
THE ROLE OF VULNERABILITYBut how do we know that women's
responses are not merely an artifact of their own greater
fear? It was our initial contention that women should be
relatively more vigilant about preventing crime because of
the caretaking role into which many women have been
socialized in our society, not just because of their
heightened feelings of vulnerability. While we cannot
directly test the effect of socialization, we can address
the impact of fear. Specifically, if a sense of
vulnerability underlies the consistent support among women
to endorse crime prevention policies, then we should find
that male-female attitudinal differences disappear at
comparable levels of security. Put differently, if the
gender coefficients found in Table 2 remain significant
after introducing controls for feelings of safety, we could
reasonably conclude that this sense of vulnerability is not
the key factor in encouraging the crime prevention
sentiments of females.
In the second column of Table 2 (and in Appendix C), we
reestimate the same regression equations found in column 1,
differing only in the sense that we now control for
respondent's fear of crime by adding Item A.1 (the
"Safe" variable) as an independent variable. The
most relevant finding for our purposes is that neither the
significance nor the magnitude of the gender coefficient
declines once we control for feelings of vulnerability,
providing strong evidence that women are relatively more
supportive of crime prevention policies for reasons that are
independent of their perceptions of personal threat. The
explanation for women's stronger support for crime
prevention efforts, in other words, seems to have less to do
with their collective sense of vulnerability than with their
commitment to protecting the vulnerable and preventing harm.
Before dismissing the possibility that fear of crime
motivates crime policy attitudes, however, it is necessary
to recall a persistent finding in the literature--viz, that
fear of crime and punitiveness are related for men, but not
for women (e.g., Feraro 1995; Mills and Bohannon 1992;
Stinchcombe et al. 1980). Given this pattern, it is possible
that the impact of safety on policy attitudes has been
"canceled out" by pooling both genders in our
analysis, as in the second column of Table 2. In Table 3,
therefore, we replicate the equations from Table 2 (second
column), but separately for male and female respondents. To
examine the precise impact of perceived safety on policy
attitudes, we report coefficients representing the Safe
variable on the various policy attitudes. The first column
includes all respondents, while the second and third columns
include only males and females, respectively. Entries are
unstandardized coefficients, which are useful for comparing
across the various regression models.
The findings of Table 3 bolster our conclusion that fear
of crime does not appear to be an important antecedent of
respondent crime policy attitudes, particularly in the case
of women: among females, not a single policy attitude is
significantly predicted by perceived safety. With men, the
threat of crime is somewhat more instrumental, as reported
in the extant literature. In each equation but one
(mandatory life prison terms to those who sell drugs to
children), the impact of safety is stronger for males than
for females. In two instances, moreover, men who are more
afraid of crime are significantly more punitive than those
who are less fearful--they are more likely to reject the
argument that reducing poverty is preferable to
"cracking down on criminals" for reducing crime,
and more likely to advocate forceful police measures (rather
than improved social programs) for the purpose of preventing
riots.
Our basic contention, however, remains unchanged. While
men may be somewhat influenced in their crime policy
attitudes by their level of fear, women are not. We remain
convinced that the best explanation for women's policy
positions is not associated with their heightened fears of
crime but, instead, that the manner in which they have been
socialized has led them, in many instances, to stronger
support for measures which protect themselves, their
families, and their neighbors from the menace of crime.
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONSAs with any argument, we are
more certain of some parts than of others. We are, for the
most part, fully convinced of the attitudinal differences
between men and women pertaining to both feelings of
vulnerability to, and remedies for, crime. As shown, women
are more likely to be concerned with the threats posed by
crime, and are more likely to emphasize prevention in their
responses to them. We want to emphasize that men and women
are not radically divided in their attitudes toward crime.
As in Conover and Sapiro's (1993) study of attitudes toward
war, we do not find women and men in wholly different camps.
Majorities of both sexes agree on most of the items we
explored. What we find are differences of degree: women are
more inclined toward protection than men, even though both
desire to control the crime problem. Men and women agree
that crime is a serious problem, but women are more afraid
and more upset by the aggression that is employed, both in
committing and in punishing it.
Quite clearly, gender is not the sole explanation of
crime-related attitudes. The full regression models (see
Appendix B) demonstrate the importance of ideology, income,
age, and other considerations. The proportion of variance
which we are unable to explain, moreover, underscores the
likelihood that these models have been underspecified and,
consequently, that important determinants of crime policy
attitudes have been ignored. Nonetheless, the consistently
important role which gender has been found to play in these
models provides us with a great deal of confidence in
concluding that men and women approach the issue quite
differently.
We can be less sure about the reasons behind these gender
differences. We earlier suggested two possible scenarios:
first, that women are more vulnerable to, and fearful of
crime and, consequently, more adamant about preventing it;
and second, that women have been socialized into a
care-taking role. The analyses in Table 2 (second column)
and 3 were designed to test the vulnerability thesis and
demonstrated to our satisfaction that fear of crime does not
drive crime policy attitudes. To the contrary, when
controlling for perceptions of fear of crime, gender
differences were equally strong as in models without the
fear variable--i.e., at constant levels of vulnerability,
men and women still differ in their responses to crime.
Almost by default, then, we have settled on a
socialization argument to explain gender differences--an
argument which outstrips the data which are available.
Minimally, then, we must wrestle with several questions
before putting too much weight on socialization as a source
of these differences.
First, it is worth asking whether the tests of the
vulnerability hypothesis were sufficiently strong. As
revealed in both Tables 1 and 2 (first column), there is
little doubt that women feel more susceptible to, and
threatened by, crime. We did not find, however, that this
heightened fear translates into stronger preferences for
preventive anti-crime policies for, as noted above,
controlling for fear does not eliminate differences between
men and women. It is important to note that, in selecting
the control variable, we deliberately selected the item
("How safe do you feel ... ?") most closely
correlated with gender, thereby providing the most stringent
test possible of the impact of holding fear constant on the
elimination of gender differences.(FN15).
Still, we acknowledge that vulnerability to crime (and
the resultant fear) can be manifest in numerous other ways
that will not be evident from items on public opinion
surveys. Marital status (i.e., being single), having
children to protect, or living in urban (rather than
suburban or rural) areas may all serve to heighten feelings
of vulnerability and, consequently, to engender preferences
for crime prevention policies. To the degree that these (or
other) life status factors are associated with gender, it is
possible both that we have underestimated the impact of fear
and that we have overestimated the role of gender. While
marital status and urban residential rates are virtually
identical for men and women,(FN16) rates of single
parenthood are not: the most recent decennial census found
that single mothers outnumber single fathers more than five
to one (U.S. Bureau of the Census 1996). Most generally,
then, we recommend caution in ruling out the possibility
that fear of crime motivates some of the attitudinal
differences which we have attributed to gender, and express
some regret at not having information pertaining to children
(and other forms of vulnerability) on the Lexington survey.
Quite conceivably, women are more preemptive with crime
because they feel more vulnerable--in ways which our
instruments have not been able to determine.
If our data are to be believed, however, the more likely
explanation is that women and men have been socialized
differently. The gender gap in attitudes toward crime which
we have found are consistent with the ethics of care and
justice suggested by Gilligan. For women, preventing crime
appears to be part of a larger concern for protecting the
vulnerable and making sure that no one is hurt. Preventing
crime is important to men as well, but they put relatively
less emphasis on prevention than punishment when asked to
choose between the two. From the female perspective,
punishment has its place, especially in the protective
context of harm to children, but it is normally more
important to avoid pain in the first place, rather than
attempting to compensate victims by causing suffering for
those who have victimized them. In this respect, women's
attitudes toward crime resemble their greater support for
social welfare spending, consumer protection, and
environmental regulation. Men's greater support for the
death penalty, the ultimate punishment, ties in well with
their support for other "force and violence"
policies, such as defense spending and military
intervention, that appeal considerably less to women.
Our data do not allow us to measure the effects of
respondents' socialization or moral voice directly. We can
merely infer that these forces are at work here. We do,
however, have some uncertainties. In the first place, we
might expect to find an interaction between age and gender--i.e.,
that women would differ attitudinally from men, but mainly
among respondents who are older and presumably socialized
under more rigidly defined sex roles. Working with a series
of interactive models, however, we were unable to uncover
such an effect: gender differences were found to be
relatively uniform regardless of age.(FN17) While there are
many ways to "explain away" such results (e.g.,
gender-based socialization patterns may persist in the
South) the results still warrant caution.
There are, moreover, numerous norms, values, beliefs, and
doctrines which are inherent to the socialization process
that have little to do with gender and which, potentially,
may have a profound impact on crime policy attitudes.
Different religions, for example, may preach more punitive,
or more forgiving, responses to law-breaking. We
unfortunately do not have the data necessary to rule out the
possibility that male-female attitudinal differences stem
from different religious orientations or, for that matter,
from other group or doctrinal affiliations.(FN18) For these
reasons, attributions of gender differences to the
socialization process must remain speculative.
Thus, while we remain quite confident of the gender
differences which we find, the precise reasons for these
differences must remain hypothetical. But this does not
minimize the importance of the findings, which extends
beyond the debate on gender. To the contrary, our results
have implications for students of both legislative and
electoral politics. In the legislative arena, they provide
grounds for questioning the often-stated assumption that
crime policy in the United States will abandon preventive
efforts and embrace increasingly punitive anti-crime
measures. At the same time that Democrats and Republicans
attempt to "out tough" one another by proposing
ever-more-harsh policies in response to crime, the public is
electing a steadily increasing number of female legislators
(see, for example, Mezey 1994). If the attitudinal
differences which we find translate into behavioral
differences when women become policymakers,(FN19) we should
see more support for preventive policies in state and
national legislatures, assuming, of course, that the
electorate continues to elect more females to make these
choices.
The electoral implications are equally intriguing. To the
degree that concerns about crime filter into the public's
voting decisions, both political parties may find themselves
in something of a dilemma. Since 1980, Republicans have been
at the short end of the so-called "gender gap"--a
phenomenon in which women (relative to men) have been
notably less supportive of both Republican candidates and
their issue positions. At the same time, the party has
benefited from citizen perceptions that their party is the
one more capable of dealing with the crime problem. In an
attempt to exploit this issue, Republicans have decried
preventive anti-crime policies as "pork,"
employing tough anti-crime rhetoric and advocating only
those policies that emphasize severe punishment for
lawbreakers. The paradox, of course, is that the very
policies which the Republicans exploit for electoral gain
(among white males) may also turn out to be the same
policies that widen the gender gap by alienating female
voters. For Democrats, the dilemma is that they must somehow
appear "tough on crime" without losing the support
of women in the electorate--women who may find the position
unattractive. In an effort to court women voters, therefore,
candidates of both parties may find it useful to champion
more crime prevention policies.
Added material.
JON HURWITZ AND SHANNON SMITHEY, UNIVERSITY OF
PITTSBURGHThe authors wish to acknowledge the assistance
provided by Bert Rockman and the University of Pittsburgh
Center for American Politics and Society, as well as that
offered by the Editors and anonymous referees. We are
particularly indebted to Mark Peffley for his involvement in
all stages of this project. The order of the authors was
determined by a coin flip.
TABLE 1 MALE-FEMALE COMPARISONS ON ATTITUDES TOWARD
SAFETY AND CRIME POLICY.
(TABLE) VARIABLE MEANS T VALUESSAFETY1. How Safe in
Neighborhood? Male 1.68 7.327(FN**) Female 2.272. How
Important is Crime? Male 2.06 1.881(FN*) Female 2.183. Crime
in Neighborhood Male 2.44 0.623 Female 2.484. Crime in
Nation Male 3.42 4.674(FN**) Female 3.75CRIME POLICY5. Spend
More for Jobs Male 2.13 4.271(FN**) Female 1.736. Drug
Rehabilitation Male 1.84 2.851(FN**) Female 1.627. Restrict
Hand Guns Male 2.31 7.375(FN**) Female 1.578. Searches for
Drugs Male 3.11 3.477(FN**) Female 2.779. Increase Prison
Terms Male 1.56 1.433 Female 1.4510. Death Penalty Male 1.69
3.276(FN**) Female 1.9911. Build More Prisons Male 2.88
0.244 Female 2.8612. Life Prison Terms Male 1.66 2.236(FN*)
Female 1.4813. Reduce Poverty Male 3.03 3.559(FN**) Female
3.3314. Rehabilitate or Punish? Male 2.49 0.820 Female
2.5715. Prevent Riots Male 1.38 3.915(FN**) Female 1.56.
FOOTNOTES* p < .05.
** p < .01.
Tests are based on samples of 465 to 501 respondents. For
the Safety items (1-4), larger values reflect feelings of
vulnerability and perceptions of increasing crime. Crime
Policy items 5-12 have been recorded so that lower values
correspond to stronger support for the policy measure. Items
13-15 present trade-offs to respondents and are coded such
that lower values represent stronger support for more
punitive preferences.
TABLE 2 REGRESSION MODELS.
(TABLE)VARIABLE GENDER b GENDER b (w/out Safe) (with
Safe)SAFETY 1. How Safe in Neighborhood? .395(FN**) 2. How
Important is Crime? .027 4. Crime in Nation .309(FN**)CRIME
POLICY 5. Spend More for Jobs -.289(FN**) -.318(FN**) 6.
Drug Rehabilitation -.200(FN**) -.182(FN*) 7. Restrict Had
Guns -.627(FN**) -.661(FN**) 8. Searches for Drugs
-.209(FN*) -.216(FN*) 10. Death Penalty .282(FN**)
.315(FN**) 12. Life Prison Terms -.134 -.110 13. Reduce
Poverty .214(FN**) .252(FN**) 15. Prevent Riots .184(FN**)
.200(FN**).
FOOTNOTES* p <.05.
** p <.01.
All models are based on 439 cases. In addition to gender,
independent variables include ideology, party
identification, age, education, political information,
income and, in the last column, safety.
TABLE 3 THE IMPACT OF FEAR OF CRIME ON POLICY ATTITUDES.
(TABLE)VARIABLE ALL MALES FEMALESSpend More for Jobs .035
.121 .052Drug Rehabilitation .080 .155(FN*) .044Restrict
Hand Guns .007 .121 .021Searches for Drugs .006 .053
.017Death Penalty .045 .119 .064Life Prison Terms .082 .006
.102Reduce Poverty .070 .148(FN*) .069Prevent Riots .008
.191(FN**) .039.
FOOTNOTES* p < .05.
** p < .01.
All models are based on 439 cases. Table entries are
unstandardized regression coefficients representing the
impact of the safety variable on policy attitudes. Other
independent variables include ideology, party
identification, age, education, political information, and
income.
FOOTNOTES1 The difference in attitudes is often much
smaller than the difference by race. As an example of this
point, see Bedyna and Lake 1994.
2 For example, interpersonal violence (Walklate 1995),
responsibility for children and aging parents (Brody 1990),
and legislative priorities (Mezey 1994).
3 This appears to be a common assumption. For example,
Stinchcombe et al. (1980: 66) note, "We punish
criminals partly because we think criminals deserve to
suffer for their wrongdoings but primarily because we think
that punishment will somehow reduce crime.".
4 Women disapprove of the use of force or violence in a
number of arenas (see Smith 1984; Shapiro and Mahajan 1986).
For example, in response to reading stories describing
instances of wife battering, male college students were much
more likely to consider a husband's use of force justified,
and female college students were much more likely to assign
culpability and advocate convictions for batterers (Pierce
and Harris 1993).
5 Prior to Gilligan, women's responses to moral dilemmas
tended to rate lower on scales of moral maturity because
they were more contextualized than the "rights and
rules" approach which characterized mens answers.
6 Gilligan has been accused of advocating a theory of
biological determinism. For this and other critiques of
Gilligan's work, see, for example, Larrabee 1993; Friedman
1987.
7 Millbrath (1984) and Steger and Witt (1989) pay
particular attention to women's greater support of
environmentalism, which they attribute to women's greater
perception of risks (to health, from pollution, and so
forth) and the environmental emphasis on "the
interconnectedness of all things (Steger and Witt 1989:
629).
8 According to the Uniform Crime Reports data published
annually by the Federal Bureau of Investigation (United
States Federal Bureau of Investigation 1995), Lexington is a
relatively typical American city in terms of its crime
rates. For the 1994 year (when our data were collected), the
rate of total crimes, violent crimes (murder, forcible rape,
robbery, and aggravated assault), and property crimes
(burglary, larceny-theft, and motor vehicle theft) per
100,000 inhabitants in the Lexington Metropolitan
Statistical Area was 5,770.1, 921.5, and 4,848.6,
respectively. These compare to national rates of 5,374.4,
716.0, and 4,658.3, respectively; rates for metropolitan
areas for the same year were: 5,894.2, 812.0, and 5,082.2,
respectively. Thus, compared to other urban areas of the
United States, Lexington has a slightly lower overall crime
rate (97.9 percent of the national urban rate) and property
crime rate (95.4 percent), but a somewhat higher rate of
violent crime (13.5 percent above the national urban rate).
Most of the discrepancy in violent crime can be explained in
terms of Lexington's high rate of aggravated assault crimes;
its property crime statistics are suppressed by the very low
rates of motor vehicle theft. While the FBI data do not
include measures of dispersion necessary for the calculation
of significance tests, an examination of the state-by-state
statistics indicates that Lexington crime rates are quite
close to both national and urban averages, considering the
wide inter-state ranges. Overall crime per 100,000, for
example, ranged from 8,250.0 (Florida) to 2,528.4 (West
Virginia). High (and low) rates for violent crime: 1,146.8
in Florida (81.8 in North Dakota); for property crimes:
7,221.4 in Arizona (2,312.6 in West Virginia).
9 The study was originally designed to explore the effect
of racial stereotypes on attitudes toward crime (see Hurwitz
and Peffley 1997). As such, only Caucasians were included in
the sample.
10 Although the representation of women in the sample
essentially mirrors the proportion in the Lexington
population, the sample overrepresents more educated and
affluent individuals. The median income in the 1990 Census
is $17,941, while the median income category in the sample
is $20,001 to $30,000. The population (and sample)
percentages of educational groups are as follows: did not
complete high school--19.8 percent in the population (7.6
percent in the sample); completed high school--23.4 percent
(24.9 percent); attended college without graduating--26.2
percent (24.7 percent); graduated college--18.6 percent
(23.9 percent); graduate work--12.0 percent (18.1 percent).
Accordingly, the sample was weighted to reflect education
levels in the Lexington population. While the weighted
sample should provide results which are more generalizable
to the Lexington population, our findings do not differ
significantly depending on whether the sample is weighted or
unweighted.
11 Survey items (and means) are included in Appendix A.
12 The average age for female and male respondents is
47.1 and 43.2 years, respectively (p < .01).
13 Among our respondents, gender is modestly correlated
with education (r = .10, p < .05), and more substantially
correlated with income (r = .21, p < .01) and our measure
of political information (r = .27, p, .01). Females, in
other words, have lower levels of education, income, and
political knowledge, relative to males.
14 Ideology and party identification are measured with
standard 7-point self-placement scales. Educated is coded
as: 1 = 0 to 4 years; 2 = 5 to 8 years; 3 = 9 to 11 years; 4
= (high school graduate; 5 = some college; 6 = college
graduate; 7 = graduate work. Political sophistication is
assessed with a 5-item knowledge index, based on the
respondent's knowledge of: (1) Al Gore's present office; (2)
the branch of government responsible for determining the
constitutionality of a law; (3) the size of the majority
necessary for the Senate and House of Representatives to
override a presidential veto; (4) the party which controlled
the House of Representatives prior to the 1992 election; and
(5) the more conservative of the political parties (Cronbach's
alpha = .739). Income is coded as: 1 = under $5,000; 2 =
$5,000 to $10,000; 3 = $10,001 to $20,000; 4 = $20,001 to
$30,000; 5 = $30,001 to $50,000; 6 = $50,001 to $100,000; 7
= over $100,000.
15 Additional analyses (unreported), using the other
three safety measures (see Appendix A, items A.2-4) both
individually and simultaneously as control variables, did
not result in diminished gender differences.
16 Census statistics indicate that 62.7 percent of men,
and 59.2 percent of women are married; and that 29.3 percent
of men, and 27.7 percent of women live in cities (U.S.
Bureau of the Census 1996).
17 We estimated both linear and nonlinear interaction
terms, given the possibility of age cohort effects. Neither
form yielded significant interaction coefficients. We also
searched, without success, for interactions between gender
and political sophistication, education, income, ideology,
and party identification.
18 We are somewhat skeptical of religious explanations of
gender differences, inasmuch as they are generally
inconsistent with our findings. Catholicism, for example,
has long taken a position against the death penalty. The
most recent American National Election Survey found higher
levels of catholicism among men (31.7 percent) than among
women (24.7), leading to the prediction that men would be
more opposed to the death penalty. We, however, find the
opposite to be true.
19 Numerous studies (Mezey 1994; Thomas 1994; Reingold
1992; among others) suggest a relationship between gender
and policy priorities. Reingold (1992), for example, finds
that female legislators are more likely than their male
counterparts to introduce and support legislation of
particular interest to women, and that female legislators of
both parties were more conscious of representing women's
interests than were their male colleagues.
REFERENCESBailey, Susan M. 1992. The AAUW Report: How
Schools Short Change Girls. Washington, DC: National
Education Association.
Baxter, Sandra, and Marjorie Lansing. 1985. Women and
Politics: the Visible Majority. Ann Arbor: University of
Michigan Press.
Belenky, Mary Field, et al. 1986 Women's Ways of Knowing:
The Development of Self, Voice and Mind. New York: Basic
Books.
Bender, Leslie. 1988. "A Lawyer's Primer on Feminist
Theory and Tort." Journal of Legal Education 38: 3-38.
Bedyna, Mary E., and Celinda C. Lake. 1994. "Gender
and Voting in the 1992 Presidential Election." In
Elizabeth Adell Cook, Sue Thomas, and Clyde Wilcox, eds.,
The Year of the Woman: Myths and Realities. Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
Blumenthal, M., et al. 1972. Justifying Violence. Ann
Arbor: Institute for Social Research.
Brody, Elaine M. 1990. Women in the Middle: The Parent
Care Years. New York: Springer.
Burt, Sandra. 1986. "Different Democracies? A
Preliminary Examination of the Political Worlds of Canadian
Men and Women." Women and Politics 6: 57-79.
Chodorow, Nancy. 1978. The Reproduction of Mothering.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Conover, Pamela Johnston. 1994. "Feminists and the
Gender Gap." In Marianne Githens, Pippa Norris, and
Joni Lovenduski, eds., Different Roles, Different Voices:
Women and Politics in the United States and Europe. New
York: HarperCollins.
Conover, Pamela Johnston, and Virginia Sapiro. 1993.
"Gender, Feminist Consciousness and War." American
Journal of Political Science 37: 1079-99.
Crawford, Mary. 1995. Talking Difference: On Gender and
Language. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Deitch, Cynthia. 1988. "Sex Differences in
Government Spending." In Carol Mueller, ed., The
Politics of the Gender Gap: The Social Construction of
Political Influence pp. 192-216. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Doyle, James A., and Michele A. Paludi. 1991. Sex and
Gender, 2nd ed. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown Publishers.
Elshtain, Jean Bethke. 1981. Public Man, Private Woman:
Women in Social and Political Thought. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Ferraro, Kenneth. 1995. Fear of Crime: Interpreting
Victim Risk. Albany: SUNY Press.
Friedman, Marilyn. 1987. "Beyond Caring: the
Demoralyzing of Gender." Canadian Journal of Philosophy
Supplementary Volume 13: 87-110.
Gilens, Martin. 1988. "Gender and Support for
Reagan: A Comprehensive Model of Presidential
Approval." American Journal of Political Science 32:
19-49.
Gilligan, Carol. 1982. In a Different Voice:
Psychological Theory and Women's Development. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Gilligan, Carol. 1993. "Reply to Critics." In
Mary Jeanne Larrabee, ed., An Ethic of Care: Feminist
Interdisciplinary Perspectives, pp. 207-14. New York:
Routledge.
Goldberg, Gertrude S., and Eleanor Kremen. 1994.
"The Feminization of Poverty: Not Only in
America." In Marianne Githens, Pippa Norris, and Joni
Lovenduski, eds., Different Roles, Different Voices: Women
and Politics in the United States and Europe. New York:
HarperCollins.
Gordon, Margaret T., and Stephanie Riger. 1989. The
Female Fear. New York: Free Press.
Hooyman, Nancy R., and Judith Gonyea. 1995. Feminist
Perspectives on Family Care: Policies for Gender Justice.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hurwitz, Jon, and Mark Peffley. 1997. "Public
Perceptions of Race and Crime: The Role of Racial
Stereotypes." American Journal of Political Science 41:
375-401.
Karmen, Andrew A. 1991. "Victims of Crime." In
Joseph F. Sheley, Criminology: A Contemporary Handbook, pp.
121-38. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Klein, Ethel. 1984. Gender Politics. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Larrabee, Mary Jeanne, ed. 1993. An Ethic of Care:
Feminist Interdisciplinary Perspectives. New York: Routledge.
McClelland, David. 1975. Power: The Inner Experience. New
York: Irvington.
Mezey, Susan Gluck. 1994. "Increasing the Number of
Women in Office: Does it Matter?" In Elizabeth Adell
Cook, Sue Thomas, and Clyde Wilcox, The Year of the Woman:
Myths and Realities. Boulder: Westview Press.
Milbrath, Lester. 1984. Environmental Vanguard for a New
Society. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Miller, Arthur. 1988. "Gender and the Vote:
1984." In Carol Mueller, ed., The Politics of the
Gender Gap: The Social Construction of Political Influence,
pp. 258-82. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Miller, J. L., Peter H. Rossi, and Jon E. Simpson. 1986.
"Perceptions of Justice: Race and Gender Differences in
Judgments of Appropriate Prison Sentences." Law and
Society Review 20: 313-34.
Mills, Carol J., and Wayne E. Bohannon. 1992. "Juror
Characteristics: To What Extent are They Related to Jury
Verdicts?" In Elliot E. Slotnick, ed., Judicial
Politics: Readings from Judicature, pp. 261-68. Chicago:
America Judicature Society.
Mueller, Carol M. 1988. The Politics of the Gender Gap:
The Social Construction of Political Influence. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Mussen, P., and N. Eisenberg-Berg. 1977. Roots of Caring,
Sharing and Helping. San Francisco: Freeman.
Norris, Pippa. 1985. "The Gender Gap in Britain and
America." Parliamentary Affairs 38: 192-201.
Pierce, Maureen C., and Richard Jackson Harris. 1993.
"The Effects of Provocation, Race and Injury
Description on Men's and Women's Perceptions of a
Wife-Battering Incident." Journal of Applied Social
Psychology 23: 767-90.
Pomper, Gerald. 1975. Voter's Choice. New York: Dodd,
Mead.
Reingold, Beth. 1992. "Concepts of Representation
Among Female and Male State Legislators." Legislative
Studies Quarterly 17: 509-37.
Research and Forecasts Inc. with Andy Friedburg. 1983.
America Afraid: How Fear of Crime Changes the Way We Live.
New York: American Library.
Rhine, Staci, Shannon Ishiyama Smithey, and Audrey
Haynes. 1994. "Cross-National Differences in Political
Attitudes: The Effects of Gender in Canada." Paper
presented at the Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political
Science Association, Chicago, IL.
Rossi, Peter H., Emily Waite, Christine E. Bose, and
Richard E. Berk. 1974. "The Seriousness of Crimes:
Normative Structure and Individual Differences."
American Sociological Review 39: 224.
Ruddick, Sara. 1989. Maternal Thinking: Towards a
Politics of Peace. Boston: Beacon Press.
Shapiro, Robert Y., and Harpreet Mahajan. 1986.
"Gender Differences in Policy Preferences: A Summary of
Trends from the 1960s to the 1980s." Public Opinion
Quarterly 50: 42-61.
Sidel, Ruth. 1986. Women and Children Last. New York:
Penguin Books.
Smith, Tom W. 1984. "The Polls: Gender and Attitudes
Toward Violence." Public Opinion Quarterly 48: 384-96.
Stanko, Elizabeth. 1990. Everyday Violence. London:
Pandora Press.
Steger, Mary Ann E., and Stephanie L. Witt. 1989.
"Gender Differences in Environmental Orientations: A
Comparison of Publics and Activists in Canada and the
U.S." Western Political Quarterly 42: 627-49.
Stinchcombe, Arthur, Rebecca Adams, Carol A. Heimer, Kim
Lane Scheppele, Tom W. Smith and D. Garth Taylor. 1980.
Crime and Punishment--Changing American Attitudes in
America. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Stoper, Emily. 1988. "The Gender Gap Concealed and
Revealed: 1936-1984." Paper presented at the annual
meeting of the Western Political Science Association, San
Francisco.
Thomas, Sue. 1994. How Women Legislate. New York: Oxford
University Press.
United States Bureau of the Census. 1996. Statistical
Abstract of the United States: 1996, 116th ed. Washington,
DC.
United States Federal Bureau of Investigation (U.S.
Department of Justice). 1995. Crime in the United States
1994: Uniform Crime Reports. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Walklate, Sandra. 1995. Gender and Crime: An
Introduction. New York: Prentice Hall.
Worden, Alissa Pollitz. 1993. "The Attitudes of
Women and Men in Policing: Testing Conventional and
Contemporary Wisdom." Criminology 31: 203-41.
Received: December 13, 1996.
Accepted: August 19, 1997.
APPENDIX A: LEXINGTON SURVEY MEASURES (AND SAMPLE MEANS)A.
SAFETY1. How safe do you feel, or would you feel, being out
alone in your neighborhood at night? Very safe, somewhat
safe, somewhat unsafe, or very unsafe? (2.03).
2. Of all the problems facing the country today, such as
the deficit, the economy, and troubles overseas, would you
say that the problem of crime is no more important than many
other problems, is one of the most important problems, or is
the single most important problem our nation faces today?
(2.13).
3. Over the past five years or so, would you say that
crime in your neighborhood has increased a lot, increased a
little, decreased, or stayed about the same? (2.47).
4. Over the past five years or so, would you say that
violent crime in our nation has increased a lot, increased a
little, decreased, or stayed about the same? (3.61).
B. CRIME POLICY ATTITUDES:(FN**)CRIME PREVENTION5. We
need to spend more money on programs to help poor people;
blacks and minorities find jobs so they can earn a decent
living without turning to crime. (1.90).
6. Creating more drug rehabilitation programs for people
in these neighborhoods.(FN*) (1.71).
7. One good way to reduce violence from hand guns is to
place more restrictions on the purchase of these weapons.
(1.88).
8. Conducting door to door searches in these
neighborhoods for illegal drugs and weapons.(FN*) (2.91).
CRIMINAL PUNISHMENT9. The best way to deal with violent
crime is to dramatically increase prison terms for people
who commit violent crimes. (1.50).
10. I strongly favor the death penalty for anyone
convicted of murder. (1.86).
11. We need to build more prisons to keep criminals off
the streets, even if it means increasing taxes. (2.87).
12. Imposing mandatory life prison terms for anyone
caught selling drugs to children. (1.56).
PREVENTION VS. PUNISHMENT13. To really reduce crime in
this country we need to focus more on reducing poverty
instead of just cracking down on criminals. (3.20).
14. In dealing with criminals, it is more important to
rehabilitate them than to punish them. (2.54).
15. People we talk to have different opinions about the
riots that occurred in Los Angeles. If you had to choose,
which do you think would be the best way to prevent
disturbances like the LA riots in the future? Do you think
there should be better social programs in poor and minority
neighborhoods or do you think police should act more
forcefully to put down any disturbances? (1.48).
FOOTNOTES* These items were preceded by the introduction:
Here are some things that law enforcement might do to deal
with the problems of drugs and crime in mostly black; white
neighborhoods in the inner city. For each one, please tell
me whether you strongly approve, somewhat approve, somewhat
disapprove, or strongly disapprove.
** With the exception of item 15 (which is dichotomous),
responses to all items in Part B are measured on four-point
scales ranging from Strongly Agree to Strongly Disagree.
APPENDIX B: COMPLETE REGRESSION MODELS.
(TABLE) 1 2 4 5 6 7Gender .204** .205** -.141** -.120**
-.272**Ideol -.086* -.179** -.140**Party Id -.101* -.082*
Age -.097* -.081* -.101* Educ -.110* -.221* Info -.137**
.177**Income -.196** .137*R2 .247 .134 .087 .091 .072
.168R2Adj .233 .118 .071 .075 .055 .153N 439 439 439 439 439
439.
(TABLE) 8 10 12 13 15Gender -.095* .133** .119** .189**Ideol
.084* .127** .128** .143** .115*Party Id .085* .144** .111*
Age .107* .116** .149** Educ .194** .188** .146* InfoIncome
-.211*R2 .104 .100 .125 .108 .117R2Adj .088 .083 .109 .092
.101N 439 439 439 439 439.
Table entries are standardized beta coefficients and
include only those variables significant at p < .05* (p
< .01**). The column heading numbers correspond to the
numbers representing the dependent variables in Table 2. The
independent variables have been coded so that lower values
correspond to: males; conservatives; Republicans; younger,
less educated, less informed, less affluent, more highly
religious, and more safe, individuals.
APPENDIX C: COMPLETE REGRESSION MODELS, INCLUDING SAFETY.
(TABLE) 5 6 7 8 10 12 13 15Gender -.156** -.109* -.286**
-.098* .149** .140** .200**Ideol -.183** -.136** .085* .122*
.124** .137** .112**Party ID -.101* -.082* .085* .143**
.111* Age .109* .109* .144** Educ .185** .180** -.109* .140*
Info .109* .187**Income .151** -.226** Safe -.102*R2 .095
.074 .173 .105 .105 .128 .116 .119R2 Adj .076 .055 .155 .086
.086 .110 .098 .101N 439 439 439 439 439 439 439 439.
Table entries are standardized beta coefficients and
include only those variables significant at p < .05* (p
< .01**). The column heading numbers correspond to the
numbers representing the dependent variables in Table 2. The
independent variables have been coded so that lower values
correspond to: males; conservatives; Republicans; younger,
less educated, less informed, less affluent, more highly
religious, and more safe, individuals.